top of page

RODENTS NAVIGATE LIKE PACIFIC ISLAND SAILORS

The navigation system rodents use is similar to the way Pacific Islanders navigate the open ocean without a compass, according to new research.

The findings correct a common misconception: that mammals’ navigation systems operate like a global positioning system, which relies on a compass-like direction sense.


“These findings offer new, compelling evidence of how our internally organized sense of direction is registered to the external world so we can navigate effectively,” observes André Fenton, a professor in the Center for Neural Science at New York University and the senior author of a paper outlining the findings in Neuron.


“Notably, how we orient in space is not akin to a GPS; rather, our direction sense is fundamentally subjective, internally organized, and intermittently registers to external landmarks,” Fenton says.


NAVIGATION MYSTERY


While many have put forth the idea that humans navigate using a GPS-like system in their brains—one that relies on a compass-like direction sense—the intricacies of this process are not clear.


However, it has been well established that the mammalian navigation system is centered in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortices—parts of the brain crucial for understanding space and memory.


Specifically, John O’Keefe, May-Britt Moser, and Edvard Moser shared the 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for their discovery of cells that signal location and distance moved—the latter of which are crucial for being oriented in space. James Ranck, a professor at SUNY Downstate Medical Center and one of the new paper’s coauthors, supplemented their work with his discovery of neurons that signal direction.


To have a better understanding of how these neuronal pieces fit in order to navigate both stationary and rotating space, Fenton, Ranck, and their colleagues conducted a series of experiments using rats.

“THESE NEW FINDINGS LINE UP WITH MOST PEOPLE’S SENSE THAT THEY CAN BECOME INTERMITTENTLY DISORIENTED AND REORIENTED, ESPECIALLY IN UNFAMILIAR SPACES…”

They created a carousel-like structure that had both stationary and moving parts. The rats were trained on a navigation task that required them to know where they were in both the moving and stationary frames. During these tasks, the scientists monitored the rats’ neurological activity, focusing on head-direction cells, which are most fundamental component of the navigation system.


However, the experimental results offered limited insights.


“The data were extremely clear and initially totally not understandable,” says Fenton. “Despite excellent navigation, the head-direction cells stopped signaling direction in either of the stationary or rotating spatial frames.”


The finding was not an uncommon one. For years, scientists have struggled to determine why laboratory rats could successfully navigate when activity in head-direction cells was quite restricted, raising questions as to how this process occurs in the brain.


NO COMPASS NECESSARY


Fenton then recalled the principles of etak navigation that he learned from a book, We, the Navigators: The Ancient Art of Landfinding in the Pacific (University of Hawaii Press, 1994), by New Zealand doctor David Lewis, that Ranck gave Fenton in the early 1990s.


From the book, Fenton learned that Pacific Islanders practice etak navigation without a compass as they move precisely between islands that are so small and far apart that they are mostly unseen during the journey, making small navigation errors deadly.


“Etak navigation is both very effective and subjectively conceptualized,” explains Fenton. “The navigator uses knowledge of the stable locations of stars in the sky and a distant visible or imagined landmark like an island.


“Despite knowing better, the navigator imagines herself stationary and the earth moving below her boat as she travels. To navigate, the navigator orients to line up herself, the etak landmark, and a star. She then journeys so that the earth-attached etak landmark moves until it is colinear with the navigator and the next orienting star. The navigator proceeds to the destination in a series of such etak stages,” Fenton says.


Re-conceptualizing navigation in the etak framework, the researchers measured the directional tuning of each head-direction neuron relative to the activity of another head-direction cell in the rats. They discovered that head-direction cells signal direction by their activity in a manner that is internally consistent, no matter if the environment is stable or rotating.


In addition, this internal direction sense intermittently registers—every 10 seconds or so—distinct landmarks in a particular space. Between registrations, the navigating rat, like the etak navigator, keeps track of where it is.


“These new findings line up with most people’s sense that they can become intermittently disoriented and reoriented, especially in unfamiliar spaces,” notes Fenton.

The research appears in the journal Neuron. The National Science Foundation, as well as a National Institute of Mental Health Training Award in Systems and Integrative Neuroscience, funded the study.

0 comments

Comments


bottom of page